, 1995; McCarron et al , 1999; Slooter et al , 1997), frontotempo

, 1995; McCarron et al., 1999; Slooter et al., 1997), frontotemporal dementia (Agosta et al., 2009), and Parkinson’s disease (Harhangi et al., 2000; Li et al., 2004; Martinez et al., 2005; Parsian et al., 2002). Furthermore, apoE4 is not rare—approximately 25% of all individuals are carriers of this allele—making the potential detrimental effects of apoE4 expression all too common. Indeed, the apoE4 allele is heavily enriched in AD patients, with 65%–80% of all AD patients carrying at least one copy

(Farrer et al., 1997). The neuropathological effects of apoE4—the least stable of the three isoforms and the most tightly associated with AD—are myriad and include Temozolomide clinical trial the following (for review, see Huang, 2010; Kim et al., 2009; Mahley et al., 2006): (1) impaired neurite outgrowth; (2) cytoskeletal disruption and hyperphosphorylation of tau; (3) mitochondrial dysfunction in neurons, including altered membrane

potential, reduced mitochondrial motility, and decreased mitochondrial respiratory enzyme levels and activity; (4) impaired synaptogenesis; (5) increased amyloid β (Aβ) production; (6) increased lysosomal Ruxolitinib cost leakage and apoptosis in neurons; (7) brain neuropathology and impaired learning and memory in mice; and (8) altered Aβ peptide clearance and/or deposition. The premise of this review is that the structural differences among the apoE isoforms determine their roles in the Cell press onset and progression of AD and other neurodegenerative diseases and that modulation of the abnormal structure of apoE4—by converting it to a more apoE3-like (or apoE2-like) structure—will reverse the apoE4-associated detrimental effects in the central nervous system (Mahley and Huang, 2012). First, however, we discuss how apoE may indirectly impact neuropathology in AD through modulation of Aβ metabolism, before moving on to present the apoE hypothesis more fully and the most recent evidence describing the

direct effects of apoE (apoE4 > apoE3 > apoE2) in the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative disorders. The amyloid hypothesis focuses on the effects of the Aβ peptide and its different assemblies in causing neuropathology, disrupting synaptic connections and forming plaques (Hardy, 2006; Palop et al., 2006; Palop and Mucke, 2010; Selkoe, 2011). Importantly, it is established that there are apoE isoform-specific effects on the Aβ pathway (Huang and Mucke, 2012; Kim et al., 2009; Selkoe, 2011) and that apoE4 expression is associated with a significant increase in amyloid plaques at earlier ages compared with apoE3 or apoE2. Furthermore, apoE4 is known to impair Aβ clearance (Bien-Ly et al., 2011; Castellano et al., 2011; Deane et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2011) and accelerate amyloid synthesis (Ye et al., 2005), as well as amyloid fibril formation and deposition (Bales et al., 1999; Bien-Ly et al., 2011; Sanan et al., 1994; Wisniewski et al., 1995).

A traumatic emotional experience inducing a lifelong anxiety diso

A traumatic emotional experience inducing a lifelong anxiety disorder would be one possibility. Evidence implicating Selleckchem Olaparib TrkB signaling in the

induction of contextual fear conditioning (Rattiner et al., 2004), an animal model mimicking some features of posttraumatic stress disorder, supports this idea. The nature of the cellular consequences of enhanced TrkB activation that underlies the pathological consequences of the brief epoch of SE is presently unclear. Determining the cellular and subcellular locale of the activated TrkB is a critical first step to elucidating the cellular consequences, a determination that can be made using high-resolution microscopy methods to localize pTrkB (Helgager et al., 2013). The present findings provide proof of concept evidence that activation of TrkB kinase is required for the induction of chronic, recurrent seizures and anxiety-like NVP-AUY922 behavior

after SE. This result provides a strong rationale for developing selective inhibitors of TrkB kinase for clinical use. That commencing TrkB kinase inhibition after SE was effective together with the short latency of access to emergency medical care of many patients with SE (Alldredge et al., 2001) enhances the feasibility of this approach to preventive therapy. The fact that just 2 weeks of treatment was sufficient to prevent TLE could minimize potential unwanted effects inherent in long-term exposure to preventive therapy. In sum, TrkB signaling provides an appealing target for developing drugs aimed at prevention of TLE. TrkBF616A and WT mice in a C57BL/6 background (Charles River) were housed under a 12 hr light/dark cycle with food and water provided ad libitum. Animals were handled according to the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of the 17-DMAG (Alvespimycin) HCl Laboratory Animals and the experiments were conducted under an approved protocol

by the Duke University Animal Care and Use Committee. Adult mice were anesthetized and a guide cannula was inserted above the right amygdala and a bipolar electrode was inserted into the left hippocampus under stereotaxic guidance (Figure S1A). After a 7-day postoperative recovery, either kainic acid (KA) (0.3 μg in 0.5 μl PBS) or vehicle (0.5 μl of PBS) was infused into the right basolateral amygdala in an awake, gently restrained animal. Hippocampal EEG telemetry (Grass Instrument) and time-locked video monitoring were performed using Harmonie software (Stellate Systems). Monitoring started at least 5 min before amygdala KA infusion for recording baseline EEG and behavioral activity. SE was typically evident electrographically and behaviorally (Mouri et al., 2008) 8–12 min after KA infusion (Figures S3A and S4A). Forty minutes after onset of KA-induced SE, diazepam (10 mg/kg, intraperitoneally [i.p.

, 2011)

The successful application of a viral, minimal p

, 2011).

The successful application of a viral, minimal promoter approach is exemplified in the Ibrutinib present study and creates novel opportunities to investigate the OT system in rats and potentially across mammalian species (Knobloch et al., 2012). In mice, combining cell-specific Cre-recombinase strains and viral delivery of loxP-flanked constructs for opsins presents an alternative approach. Taken together with previous findings from the same group and others (Viviani et al., 2011 and Ciocchi et al., 2010), the findings of the present study suggest the existence of distinct routes by which fear signals flow through the central amygdala. This signals use previously unknown, spatially overlapping but nonetheless functionally segregated neuronal networks that underlie different components of the fear response, e.g., behavioral versus autonomic or active

versus passive fear expression. These microcircuits consist of neurons characterized by distinct expression of marker proteins such as neuromodulators or their receptors, which in turn directly impact on cellular function and subsequent circuit output. We now have the possibility of genetically targeting and interfering with selected circuit elements to not only characterize anatomy and connectivity, but also to investigate their specific function (Haubensak et al., 2010 and Letzkus et al., 2011) and thus to dissect neuronal circuitry

underlying complex behavior with unprecedented precision. “
“The gaze shifts we make four or five times per second are crucial to our exploration of a visual find protocol scene. They somehow succeed in repeatedly and accurately repositioning the eyes so that the most acute region of each retina (the fovea) acquires the target of greatest interest. For foveate animals like us, this is where visually guided behavior begins; that is, with the selection of a peripheral visual stimulus for further visual processing. One refers to this behavior as the overt orienting of visual attention because the selection of the target culminates in an observable movement of the see more eyes (or the eyes and the head) to acquire a specific target. Thus, for example, before crossing the street we might shift our gaze to a car moving toward us while ignoring another car moving away from us, the gaze shift being exclusively driven by velocity of the target car. This example depicts the more mundane, or one might say pedestrian, form of visual attention. However, this is not the type of attention most often studied by those who seek to identify its neural basis. The type of attention typically studied by neurophysiologists is the kind devoid of changes in gaze, namely covert attention, in which the only measurable effects on behavior are perceptual. As several 19th-century scientists (e.g.

, 2012; O’Roak et al , 2012; Sanders et al , 2012) Among the 1,0

, 2012; O’Roak et al., 2012; Sanders et al., 2012). Among the 1,000 families assessed by the four studies, the rate of de novo loss-of-function (LoF) variation was consistently found to be significantly higher in cases compared to controls, allowing for the development of rigorous statistical approaches to identifying specific risk genes. Indeed, six ASD genes were identified, CHD8, DYRK1A, GRIN2B, KATNAL2, POGZ, and GSK-J4 SCN2A, because they carried recurrent,

highly damaging de novo events. While SCN2A has been previously implicated in epilepsy, none of these genes were known to carry ASD risk. Another key finding, one that will prove useful for gene discovery, was that roughly half of all de novo LoF mutations seen in ASD probands fall in ASD genes, with about 12% of ASD subjects

showing a de novo LoF mutation. These WES studies found a background rate of missense de novo variation that is more than 10-fold higher than that for LoF alleles. These missense changes undoubtedly include risk alleles; however, only a 5%–10% excess of such mutations was found in ASD cohorts, a difference that did not reach significance collectively across studies. Accordingly, it is not yet possible to confidently assign risk to this broad category of mutation, nor to establish an agreed upon threshold for the significance of observing “recurrent” de novo missense mutations in a given gene. Given the relevance of LoF alleles, this difficulty surely reflects the signal-to-noise problems engendered by neutral background variation and the difficulties that attend differentiating the subset of truly functional missense check details variations. The interpretation of case-control exome sequencing has also not been as straightforward as family studies evaluating de novo LoF events. For example, WES of a sample of 1,000 cases and 1,000 controls and inspection of the six novel ASD genes just described showed, in hindsight, only a slight excess of LoF mutations in KATNAL2 and CHD8 in cases, a difference that did not approach statistical significance

( Neale et al., 2012). Indeed, across the entire genome, no genes were found to harbor a sufficiently large excess of rare alleles in cases versus controls to support a significant association after accounting heptaminol for multiple comparisons (Liu et al., personal communication). These results are consistent with the multiple lines of evidence supporting a large number of ASD risk genes scattered throughout the genome. Methods to extract signal from case-control studies, alone and in combination with de novo data, are rapidly evolving. Still, it seems reasonable to conclude that large studies, involving tens of thousands of subjects, will be necessary to identify risk loci using standard analyses of mutation burden in case-control samples. The path forward is either WES or WGS in large cohorts.

A region of inferior parietal lobule was also found to track esti

A region of inferior parietal lobule was also found to track estimation uncertainty. Such a finding relates to previous studies that have assessed neural correlates of ambiguity during economic decision-making (Bach et al., 2011 and Huettel et al., 2006). In those studies, subjects were provided with partial information regarding the probabilities associated with obtaining a reward outcome and could not improve

their estimate of those probabilities through sampling. In contrast, in our case, estimation uncertainty reduces over trials as the number of samples of an option increases provided there is no jump in the outcome probabilities. 3-Methyladenine mw Although findings of neural overlap must be treated with caution, by showing that ambiguity and estimation uncertainty do appear to engage at least partly overlapping regions, our finding suggests that the two may engage similar underlying computational processes. Now turning to risk, we found significant correlations with this variable in inferior frontal gyrus as well as a region of lingual gyrus bilaterally.

In previous studies describing neural representations of risk, activity has also been reported in the inferior frontal gyrus (Huettel et al., 2005) and the adjacent anterior insula (Huettel et al., 2005; Preuschoff et al., 2008). Other studies have reported activations in additional brain regions not found at our whole-brain-corrected threshold, including the anterior cingulate cortex (Christopoulos et al., DAPT price 2009) and the intraparietal sulcus. Furthermore, we found activity in the lingual gyrus, an area typically not found to correlate with risk per se, although Callan et al. (2009) found that lingual gyrus is involved in tracking resolution of uncertainty, and Bruguier et al. (2010) reported enhanced lingual gyrus activation when insider trading risk increased in the context of a financial market. One potential account for the differences in activation

patterns found here is that because we are modeling other uncertainty components at the same time and therefore accounting for confounding variance, this confers found a greater sensitivity to uncover signals specifically pertaining to risk on the present study, as opposed to those confounding variables. Furthermore, in many previous studies assessing risk perception, reward probabilities were presented explicitly in a descriptive fashion (Christopoulos et al., 2009, Huettel et al., 2005 and Preuschoff et al., 2008; also see d’Acremont et al., 2009), while in our task, neural representations of risk are acquired through direct sampling from a distribution of reward. Thus, putative differences between neural systems involved in descriptive versus experiential learning may account partially for involvement of distinct brain areas to those found in studies on risk representations in descriptive tasks. Finally, we observed activity in cuneus correlating with the learning rate.

The decrease in total,

but not synaptic, surface GluA1 af

The decrease in total,

but not synaptic, surface GluA1 after glycine treatment in the DKD cells suggests that the extrasynaptic AMPARs in the DKD cells may be relatively unstable and more susceptible to endocytosis. Consistent with this hypothesis, the constitutive endocytosis of GluA1-containing AMPARs increased after LRRTM DKD and returned to basal levels with expression of LRRTM2 click here (Figure S7). Our results suggest that LRRTMs are required for LTP at synapses on early postnatal CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo and on cultured neurons in vitro. However, the effects of LRRTM DKD on basal AMPAR-mediated synaptic responses in vivo depend on the maturational state of the synapses (Soler-Llavina et al., 2011). Furthermore, NL1 KD was reported to impair LTP at early postnatal but not at mature synapses on CA1 pyramidal neurons (Shipman and Nicoll, 2012), although the NL1 JQ1 concentration KO does not cause a major impairment in LTP (Blundell et al., 2010), suggesting that NL1 is not required during development to render synapses competent for LTP. These findings raise the possibility that LRRTMs may not play a critical role in mediating LTP at mature synapses but instead that the in vivo LRRTM DKD

at P0 may prevent synapses from reaching a maturational state necessary to support LTP. To address this possibility, we injected the LRRTM DKD lentivirus into the CA1 region of P21 mice, a time point at which synapses have largely matured, and then performed recordings in slices prepared 14–18 days later (Figures 4A and 4B). P35–P39 control neurons expressed robust LTP (Figure 4C), whereas LTP was dramatically reduced in DKD neurons (Figures 4D and 4E; control = 2.1 ± 0.18, n = 13 cells; DKD = 1.26 ± 0.11, n = 12 cells). Furthermore, expression of LRRTM2 rescued LTP (Figures 4F and 4G; DKD-LRR2 = 2.0 ± 0.30, n = 7 cells) as did expression of LRR2Ex (Figures 4H and 4I; control = 2.14 ± 0.41, n = 5 cells; DKD-LRR2Ex = 2.08 ± 0.33, PD184352 (CI-1040) n = 6 cells). Despite decades of effort,

the molecular mechanisms underlying classic NMDAR-dependent LTP at excitatory synapses on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons remain poorly understood. Indeed, recent work points out the need to re-examine current hypotheses about LTP mechanisms (Granger et al., 2013, Lee et al., 2013 and Volk et al., 2013) and the importance of testing the role of novel proteins. Here we investigated the role of LRRTMs (Laurén et al., 2003 and Linhoff et al., 2009) in standard LTP because, like NLs, LRRTMs form an adhesion complex with Nrxs (de Wit et al., 2009, Ko et al., 2009, Ko et al., 2011 and Siddiqui et al., 2010), their in vivo KD during early postnatal development affects AMPAR-mediated, but not NMDAR-mediated, synaptic responses (Soler-Llavina et al., 2011), and they may directly bind to AMPAR subunits (de Wit et al., 2009 and Schwenk et al.

At intermediate distances (10-30 μm), CF responses were still enh

At intermediate distances (10-30 μm), CF responses were still enhanced on average, but to a lower degree than at ROI-1. In both types of experiments,

local amplification of dendritic CF responses was used as a measure of excitability changes, because CF signaling provides large, widespread signals that can be recorded at multiple dendritic locations. In addition to its use as an indicator of dendritic plasticity, this location-specific amplification process is physiologically interesting, because an enhancement of the instructive CF signal and the associated calcium transient could locally affect the LTD/LTP balance at nearby PF synapses (Ohtsuki et al., 2009). It has previously been demonstrated in vivo that brief high-frequency bursts constitute Everolimus a typical granule cell response to sensory stimulation (Chadderton et al., 2004).

Thus, the PF burst pattern used (5 pulses at 50 Hz; repeated at 5 Hz for 3 s) likely provides a physiological input pattern, suggesting that the spatial Selleckchem OSI744 restriction of dendritic plasticity reported here (on average no amplification at distances of > 30 μm from the conditioned site) reflects a physiologically relevant degree of localization. It should be noted, however, that this finding does not exclude the possibility that dendritic excitability changes can be even more MycoClean Mycoplasma Removal Kit spatially restricted. In CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons, local changes in A-type K channels result in long-term adjustments of branch coupling strength that have been suggested to play a role in the storage of specific input patterns (Losonczy et al., 2008 and Makara et al., 2009). Another study showed that A-type K channels and SK channels

play complementary roles in limiting dendritic responses to the stimulated branch (Cai et al., 2004). However, there is a fundamental difference in the way that SK channels and voltage-gated K channels control dendritic responsiveness. SK channels are exclusively activated by calcium and, in turn, regulate the amplitude and kinetics of EPSPs and curtail spine calcium transients (Belmeguenai et al., 2010, Lin et al., 2008 and Ngo-Anh et al., 2005). Thus, SK channel activation is part of a negative feedback loop that is closely tied to calcium signaling and provides a unique brake mechanism to influence dendritic processing. Our data provide the first demonstration that the gain of this dendritic brake mechanism may be adjusted in an activity-dependent way. Moreover, we show that this form of plasticity of dendritic IE can be restricted to selectively activated compartments of the dendrite.

, 1993 and Takahashi et al , 1994) Subsequent work has shown tha

, 1993 and Takahashi et al., 1994). Subsequent work has shown that G1 lengthening acts to promote neurogenesis during development of the mammalian cerebral cortex (Lange et al., 2009) and is not simply a passive consequence of the switch to neurogenesis. More recently, it has been found that the increase in G1 is due to an increase in the genesis of basal progenitor cells that have a relatively long G1 phase from apical progenitor cells that have a shorter G1 phase (Arai et al., 2011). In addition, an extended S phase is found in cortical stem cells that expand the stem cell pool as opposed to click here those destined to generate

neurons. The latter observation has been suggested to reflect the greater need for careful quality control of DNA replication in expanding stem cells than in stem cells about to undergo a terminal division to generate two postmitotic neurons (Arai et al., 2011). Optic lobe neuroepithelial cells also undergo a transient cell-cycle arrest prior to adopting the neuroblast fate (Hofbauer and Campos-Ortega, 1990, Orihara-Ono et al., 2011 and Reddy et al., 2010). G1 arrest is induced through downregulation of the Fat-Hippo signaling pathway (Orihara-Ono et al., 2011 and Reddy et al., 2010). Expression of a constitutively

activated form of Yorkie (Yrk), a transcription factor controlled by Fat-Hippo signaling, prevents the cell-cycle arrest and blocks the transition from neuroepithelial cell to neuroblast. Similarly, in the chicken neural tube overexpression of Yes-associated this website protein (YAP, the vertebrate ortholog of Yrk) results in the expansion of the neural progenitor pool at the expense of differentiating cells (Cao et al., 2008). Recent results suggest that FatJ, the closest vertebrate homolog to Drosophila Fat, regulates Yap in the vertebrate neural tube ( Van Hateren et al., 2011).

In the Drosophila optic lobe as well as in the chicken neural tube YAP/Yrk positively regulates cell-cycle regulators to accelerate cell-cycle progression during early to mid-G1. Overall, it others is clear that the complex interplay between cell-cycle regulation and cell-fate determination systems is a common feature of neural stem cells in vertebrates and invertebrates. During asymmetric cell division in some cell types, the nonrandom segregation of mother versus daughter centrosomes has been observed to correlate with differences in cell fate (reviewed by Macara and Mili, 2008). The functional importance of centrosomes in neural stem cell self-renewal is evident from primary microcephaly (MCPH), an autosomal-recessive human condition in which the entire brain, and to a greater degree the cerebral cortex, are reduced in size (Thornton and Woods, 2009). Of the eight known MPCH loci, disease-causing mutations have been found in six genes, all of which encode proteins found in centrosomes (such as ASPM; Bond et al.

On average, the contralateral excitatory synaptic response (measu

On average, the contralateral excitatory synaptic response (measured around the best frequency and at 70 dB SPL) was stronger than the binaural excitatory response (p < 0.01, paired t test), whereas the

contralateral inhibitory synaptic response was not different from its binaural counterpart (p > 0.2, paired t test) ( Figure 4D). In contrast, ipsilateral excitatory and inhibitory inputs were both weaker than their binaural counterparts (p < 0.01, paired t test), but the difference was far smaller for inhibition than excitation ( Figure 4D). Figure 4E plots the scaling factor for the contralateral-to-binaural synaptic response transformation. In all the recorded cells, the scaling factor for excitation was below 1, indicating click here a suppressive effect despite the fact that ipsilateral stimulation alone evoked excitation. The scaling factor for inhibition was close to 1, indicating a much weaker modulation of inhibition by ipsilateral stimulation. As for receptive field shape, binaural synaptic TRFs closely resembled their contralateral counterparts, as demonstrated by their similar bandwidths ( Figure 4F) and CFs (

Figures S3C and S3D). On the other hand, ipsilateral synaptic TRFs were significantly narrower than Caspase inhibitor their binaural counterparts ( Figure 4F). Together, these summaries strengthen the notion that ipsilateral ear input serves a modulatory function in generating binaural spike responses primarily by scaling mafosfamide down contralaterally evoked excitatory input. To test whether the observed

scaling of excitatory input contributes to the apparent linear transformation of the contralateral into binaural spike response, we employed a conductance-based neuron model (Liu et al., 2011, Zhou et al., 2012a, Zhou et al., 2012b and Sun et al., 2013). Figures 5A and 5B show the tone-evoked excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs at 70 dB SPL for a typical ICC neuron. We fit the frequency distribution of synaptic response amplitudes with a Gaussian function (Figures 5C and 5D). The normalized Gaussian functions for binaural and contralateral synaptic responses superimposed well (Figures 5C and 5D, inset), indicating little difference in tuning shape and again supporting the notion of scaling. We utilized these Gaussian fits to simulate frequency tuning of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs in our model. For simplicity, the best frequencies of excitation and inhibition were chosen to be the same (see Figures S3C and S3D), and their tuning shapes were both symmetric (Figure 5E). Tone-evoked excitatory and inhibitory conductances (Figure 5E, inset) were simulated by fitting experimental data with an alpha function (see Experimental Procedures).

We performed immunostaining against the vesicular glutamate trans

We performed immunostaining against the vesicular glutamate transporter (VGlut1), a presynaptic marker for glutamatergic synapses, and, in a separate set of animals, against VGAT and gephyrin, pre- and postsynaptic markers for GABAergic synapses. We found that spines were juxtaposed to VGlut1 positive structures at a level much higher than chance (Figures 1D and 1E), but not to VGAT- and gephyrin-positive structures

(Figure 1E). In fact, spines colocalized with inhibitory synaptic markers (either one or both markers) at levels significantly lower than chance. Together, these data suggest that the majority of spines on the dendrites of inhibitory neurons carry synapses, and that most of these are learn more from excitatory, but not inhibitory, presynaptic neurons. Having shown that spines http://www.selleckchem.com/products/Bafilomycin-A1.html of inhibitory neurons colocalize

with markers for excitatory synapses, we next explored if these synapses carried functional receptors. In acute slices of visual cortex, we used whole cell voltage clamp recordings of GFP-expressing spiny inhibitory neurons to measure excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) evoked by focal two-photon glutamate uncaging. Glutamate uncaging immediately adjacent to spines consistently elicited EPSCs in all spines tested (n = 15, Figures 1F and 1G). Uncaging performed at the same distance from the dendritic shaft in the absence of a spine consistently evoked far smaller responses (Figures 1F and 1G), suggesting that currents

were likely elicited through synapses on spines and not the result of glutamate diffusion to synapses located elsewhere. Together, these data show that dendritic spines of inhibitory also neurons carry functional glutamatergic receptors. For excitatory pathways, it has been shown that dendritic spines on cortical pyramidal neurons are not stable over time. Even under baseline conditions, new spines grow and existing ones disappear (Grutzendler et al., 2002, Hofer et al., 2009, Holtmaat et al., 2006, Keck et al., 2008, Majewska et al., 2006, Trachtenberg et al., 2002 and Zuo et al., 2005). To examine whether interneuron spines show a similar behavior, we used repeated two-photon imaging in the monocular visual cortex of adult (p80-100) GAD65-GFP mice (López-Bendito et al., 2004 and Wierenga et al., 2010). We imaged the dendrites of inhibitory neurons located in layers 1 and 2/3 (0–200 μm below the pial surface). Previous work has reported modifications of dendritic branch tips of inhibitory neurons in visual cortex (Lee et al., 2006 and Lee et al., 2008), which increase after plasticity (Chen et al., 2011); however, for the types of interneurons labeled in the GAD65-GFP mouse line used here, we found that dendrites were largely stable. In contrast, and similar to excitatory neurons (Grutzendler et al., 2002, Hofer et al., 2009, Holtmaat et al., 2006, Keck et al., 2008, Majewska et al., 2006, Trachtenberg et al., 2002 and Zuo et al.